Читать книгу SuperCooperators - Roger Highfield - Страница 14

QUEST FOR THE EVOLUTION
OF COOPERATION

Оглавление

Mathematics, rightly viewed, possesses not only truth, but supreme beauty—a beauty cold and austere, like that of sculpture, without appeal to any part of our weaker nature, without the gorgeous trappings of painting or music, yet sublimely pure, and capable of a stern perfection such as only the greatest art can show.

—Bertrand Russell, Study of Mathematics

My overall approach to reveal and understand the mechanisms of cooperation is easy to explain, even if my detailed workings might appear mysterious. I like to take informal ideas, instincts, even impressions of life and render them into a mathematical form. Mathematics allows me to chisel down into messy, complicated issues and—with judgment and a little luck—reveal simplicity and grandeur beneath. At the heart of a successful mathematical model is a law of nature, an expression of truth that is capable of generating awe in the same way as Michelangelo’s extraordinary sculptures, whose power to amaze comes from the truth they capture about physical beauty.

Legend has it that when asked how he had created David, his masterpiece, Michelangelo explained that he simply took away everything from the block of marble that was not David. A mathematician, when confronted by the awesome complexity of nature, also has to hack away at a wealth of observations and ideas until the very essence of the problem becomes clear, along with a mathematical idea of unparalleled beauty. Just as Michelangelo wanted his figures to break free from the stone that imprisoned them, so I want mathematical models to take on a life beyond my expectations, and work in circumstances other than those in which they were conceived.

Michelangelo sought inspiration from the human form, notably the male nude, and also from ideas such as Neoplatonism, a philosophy that regards the body as a vessel for a soul that longs to return to God. Over the few centuries that science has been trying to make sense of nature, the inspiration for mathematical representations of the world has changed. At first, the focus was more on understanding the physical world. Think of how Sir Isaac Newton used mathematics to make sense of motion, from the movement of the planets around the sun to the paths of arrows on their way to a target. To the amazement of many, Newton showed that bodies on Earth and in the majestic heavens were governed by one and the same force—gravity—even though planets are gripped in an orbit while objects like arrows and apples drop to the ground.

Today, the models of our cosmos are also concerned with biology and society. Among the eddies and ripples of that great river of ideas that has flowed down the generations to shape the ways in which scientists model these living aspects of the world are the powerful currents generated by Charles Darwin (1809–1882), who devised a unifying view of life’s origins, a revolutionary insight that is still sending out shock waves today.

Darwin worked slowly and methodically, using his remarkable ability to make sense of painstaking studies he had conducted over decades, to conclude that all contemporary species have a common ancestry. He showed that the process of natural selection was the major mechanism of change in living things. Because reproduction is not a perfect form of replication, there is variation and with this diversity comes the potential to evolve. But equally, as the game of Chinese Whispers (also known as Gossip or Telephone) illustrates, without a way of selecting changes that are meaningful—a sentence that makes sense—the result is at best misleading and at worst a chaotic babble. Darwin came up with the idea that a trait will persist over many generations only if it confers an evolutionary advantage, and that powerful idea is now a basic tenet of science.

Darwin’s message is simple and yet it helps to generate boundless complexity. There exists, within each and every creature, some information that can be passed from one generation to the next. Across a population, there is variation in this information. Because when there are limited resources and more individuals are born than can live or breed, there develops a struggle to stay alive and, just as important, to find a mate. In that struggle to survive, those individuals who bear certain traits (kinds of information) fail and are overtaken by others who are better suited to their environs. Such inherited differences in the ability to pass genes down the generations—natural selection—mean that advantageous forms become more common as the generations succeed. Only one thing counts: survival long enough to reproduce.

Darwin’s theory to explain the diverse and ever-changing nature of life has been buttressed by an ever-increasing wealth of data accumulated by biologists. As time goes by, the action of selection in a given environment means that important differences can emerge during the course of evolution. As new variations accumulate, a lineage may become so different that it can no longer exchange genes with others that were once its kin. In this way, a new species is born. Intriguingly, although we now call this mechanism “evolution,” the word itself does not appear in The Origin of Species.

Darwin himself was convinced that selection was ruled by conflict. He wrote endlessly about the “struggle for existence” all around us in nature. His theme took on a life of its own as it was taken up and embellished with gusto by many others. Nature is “red in tooth and claw,” as Tennyson famously put it when recalling the death of a friend. The catchy term “survival of the fittest” was coined in 1864 by the philosopher Herbert Spencer, a champion of the free market, and this signaled the introduction of Darwinian thinking into the political arena too.

Natural selection is after all about competition, dog-eat-dog and winner takes all. But Darwin was of course talking about the species that was the best adapted to an environment, not necessarily the strongest. Still, one newspaper concluded that Darwin’s work showed that “might is right & therefore that Napoleon is right & every cheating tradesman is also right.” Darwin’s thinking was increasingly abused to justify the likes of racism and genocide, to explain why white colonialists triumphed over “inferior” native races, to breed “superior” humans and so on. These abuses are, in a twisted and depressing way, a testament to the power of his ideas.

But, as I have already stressed, competition is far from being the whole story. We help each other. Sometimes we help strangers too. We do it on a global scale with charities such as Oxfam, which helps people in more than seventy countries, and the Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation, which supports work in more than one hundred nations. We do it elaborately, with expensive celebrity-laden fund-raising dinners in smart venues. We are also charitable to animals. Why? This may look like an evolutionary loose end. In fact it is absolutely central to the story of life.

When cast in an evolutionary form, the Prisoner’s Dilemma shows us that competition and hence conflict are always present, just as yin always comes with yang. Darwin and most of those who have followed in his giant footsteps have talked about mutation and selection. But we need a third ingredient, cooperation, to create complex entities, from cells to societies. I have accumulated a wide range of evidence to show that competition can sometimes lead to cooperation. By understanding this, we can explain how cells, and multicellular organisms such as people, evolved, and why they act in the complicated ways that they do in societies. Cooperation is the architect of living complexity.

To appreciate this, we first need to put evolution itself on a firmer foundation. Concepts such as mutation, selection, and fitness only become precise when bolted down in a mathematical form. Darwin himself did not do this, a shortcoming that he was only too aware of. In his autobiography, he confessed his own inability to do sums: “I have deeply regretted that I did not proceed far enough at least to understand something of the great leading principles of mathematics; for men thus endowed seem to have an extra sense.” He seemed aware that more rigor was required to flesh out the implications of his radical ideas about life. He regarded his mind “as a machine for grinding general laws out of large collections of facts.” But even Darwin yearned for a more “top down” approach, so he could conjure up more precise laws to explain a great mass of data. He needed a mathematical model.

The modern understanding of the process of inheritance is now called “Mendelian,” in honor of Gregor Mendel, who had settled for being a monk after failing his botany exams at the University of Vienna. By sorting out the results of crossing round and wrinkly peas, Mendel revealed that inheritance is “particulate” rather than “blending.” Offspring inherit individual instructions (genes) from their parents such that round and wrinkly parents produce either round or wrinkly offspring and not something in between. What is often overlooked in his story is that Mendel was a good student of mathematics. The great geneticist and statistician Sir Ronald Fisher went so far as to call him “a mathematician with an interest in biology.” Mendel uncovered these rules of inheritance because he was motivated by a clear mathematical hypothesis, even to the extent of ignoring ambiguous results that did not fit. Had Mendel conducted an open-minded statistical analysis of his results, he might not have been successful.

A simple equation to show the effect of passing genes down the generations was found in 1908 by G. H. Hardy, a cricket-loving Cambridge mathematician who celebrated the artistry of his subject in his timeless book A Mathematician’s Apology. In an unusual reversal of the usual roles, the work of this pure mathematician was generalized by the German doctor Wilhelm Weinberg to show the incidence of genes in a population. Robert May (now Lord May of Oxford) once went so far as to call the Hardy-Weinberg law biology’s equivalent of Newton’s first law. Thanks to Hardy and Weinberg we now had a mathematical law that applied across a spectrum of living things.

This attempt to model how inheritance works in nature was extended in seminal investigations conducted in the 1920s and 1930s by a remarkable trio. First, Sir Ronald Fisher, whose extraordinary ability to visualize problems came from having to be tutored in mathematics as a child without the aid of paper and pen, due to his poor eyesight. There was also the mighty figure of J. B. S. Haldane, an aristocrat and Marxist who once edited the Daily Worker. I will return to Haldane in chapter 5. The last of this remarkable trio was Sewall Wright, an American geneticist who was fond of philosophy, that relative of mathematics (forgive me for cracking the old joke about the difference: while mathematicians need paper, pencil, and a wastepaper basket, philosophers need only paper and pencil).

Together, this threesome put the fundamental concepts of evolution, selection, and mutation in a mathematical framework for the first time: they blended Darwin’s emphasis on individual animals competing to sire the next generation with Mendel’s studies of how distinct genetic traits are passed down from parent to offspring, a combination now generally referred to as the synthetic view of evolution, the modern synthesis, or neo-Darwinian. With many others, I have also extended these ideas by looking at the Prisoner’s Dilemma in evolving populations to come up with the basic mechanisms that explain how cooperation can thrive in a Darwinian dog-eat-dog world.

Over the years I have explored the Dilemma, using computer models, mathematics, and experiments to reveal how cooperation can evolve and how it is woven into the very fabric of the cosmos. In all there are five mechanisms that lead to cooperation. I will discuss each one of them in the next five chapters and then, in the remainder of the book, show how they offer novel insights into a diverse range of issues, stretching from straightforward feats of molecular cooperation to the many and intricate forms of human cooperation.

I will examine the processes that paved the way to the emergence of the first living things and the extraordinary feats of cooperation that led to multicellular organisms, along with how cellular cooperation can go awry and lead to cancer. I will outline a new theory to account for the tremendous amount of cooperation seen in the advanced social behavior of insects. I will move on to discuss language and how it evolved to be the glue that binds much of human cooperation; the “public goods” game, the biggest challenge to cooperation today; the role of punishment; and then networks, whether of friends or acquaintances, and the extraordinary insights into cooperation that come from studying them. Humans are SuperCooperators. We can draw on all the mechanisms of cooperation that I will discuss in the following pages, thanks in large part to our dazzling powers of language and communication. I also hope to explain why I have come to the conclusion that although human beings are the dominant cooperators on Earth, man has no alternative but to evolve further, with the help of the extraordinary degree of control that we now exert over the modern environment. This next step in our evolution is necessary because we face serious global issues, many of which boil down to a fundamental question of survival. We are now so powerful that we could destroy ourselves. We need to harness the creative power of cooperation in novel ways.

SuperCooperators

Подняться наверх