Читать книгу Doing Focus Groups - Rosaline Barbour - Страница 23
Claims and challenges in focus
ОглавлениеThe varying terms employed by different writers (or disciplinary representatives) who label their method as involving ‘group interviews’, ‘focus group interviews’ or ‘focus group discussions’ reflect different assumptions as to the purpose of the study and the nature and status of the data that are generated. Some usages emphasize the capacity of focus groups to access ‘views’ and may, at times, implicitly reference the quantitative tradition of data collection, privileging the notion of fixed ‘attitudes’. Others focus, either implicitly or explicitly, on a social constructionist model that emphasizes the potential of focus groups to elicit exchanges between participants as they co-construct perspectives and responses. (This is discussed in more detail in Chapter 2 in relation to ‘epistemological and ontological’ underpinnings of focus group research.) The definition that I continue to apply (first outlined in Chapter 1) is suitably broad to afford space for both applied and more theoretical usages of focus groups: ‘Any group discussion may be called a focus group as long as the researcher is actively encouraging of, and attentive to, the group interaction’ (Kitzinger and Barbour, 1999, p. 20).
Being actively encouraging of group interaction relates, most obviously, to running the focus group discussion and ensuring that participants talk amongst themselves rather than interacting only with the researcher, or ‘moderator’. However, it also relates to the preparation required in developing a topic guide and selecting stimulus materials that will encourage interaction, as well as to decisions made with regard to group composition, in order to ensure that participants will have enough in common with each other to make discussion seem appropriate, yet have sufficiently varying experiences or perspectives in order to allow for some debate or differences of opinion. Likewise, although being attentive to group interaction refers to the process of moderating discussions, with the researcher picking up on differences in views or emphasis of participants and exploring these, it also relates to the importance of paying attention to group interaction: to group dynamics and to the activities engaged in by the group – whether this be forming a consensus, developing an explanatory framework, interpreting health promotion messages, or weighing up competing priorities. Later chapters in this book are concerned with providing advice on all of these aspects of research design, the running of focus groups and analyzing the data generated.
Focus groups, in common with other qualitative methods, excel at providing insights into process rather than outcome. This, however, is sometimes overlooked by researchers who employ focus groups as a method. A common usage is the so-called ‘nominal group technique’, which has proved so popular in health services research. Literally meaning ‘a researcher-convened rather than naturally occurring group’ – a group in name only – the most common variant of ‘nominal groups’ involves employing a ranking exercise in order to encourage participants to determine their priorities. While I would contend that important insights can be gained by paying detailed attention to the discussion generated during the process of debating and weighing up competing priorities, many proponents of this approach concentrate their efforts, instead, on the outcome of such deliberations. A notable exception is provided by Demant and Ravn (2010) who capitalized on the data-generating potential afforded by a ranking exercise to allow young Danish people to articulate, explore, debate and refine their ideas about drugs and risk. (This study is discussed in more detail in Chapter 6 on generating data.)
Although some commentators have bemoaned the tendency of focus groups to produce consensus, others, such as Myers and Macnaghten (1999), have pointed out that, in the event, many groups do not develop a consensus, and that, moreover, it is the interchanges between participants that constitute the most valuable data for the researcher attempting to gain insight into group processes; not the outcome of the discussion.
All comments made during focus groups are highly dependent upon context and are contingent upon group members’ responses to others’ contributions and the dynamics of that particular group. Lehoux et al. (2006), for example, have questioned the emerging consensus about what constitutes the patient’s view, highlighting the extent to which this is context-dependent and a product of the focus group discussion setting. (This issue is re-visited in Chapters 8 and 9 which discuss the use of group interaction in the process of analysis of interaction in groups.) As Billig (1991) reminds us, views expressed in focus groups are highly specific and are ‘bound up in the argument [that is] happening’. It is misguided to attempt to extrapolate from focus group discussion to attempt to measure individuals’ attitudes. Although not explicitly utilizing focus groups as a ‘back-door’ route to survey data, some researchers, nevertheless, may express frustration regarding the perceived ‘slipperiness’, or elusiveness, of views throughout focus group discussions. Participants frequently change their minds about issues in the course of discussion, particularly where focus groups address a topic to which they had not previously paid a great deal of attention. Researchers are in danger of treating views as if they exist independently of our focus group discussions, when it would be more helpful to regard the research encounter itself as a ‘site of performance’ (Brannen and Pattman, 2005, p. 53). Virtually without fail, close analysis of focus group discussions highlights inconsistencies and contradictions. This is a problem only if one views attitudes as fixed. As David Morgan (1988) has observed: ‘Focus groups are useful when it comes to investigating what participants think, but they excel at uncovering why participants think as they do’ (1988, p. 25).
If looked at through a different lens, these so-called ‘slippery’ views can be perceived as a resource rather than as a problem. Focus groups excel at allowing us to study the processes of attitude formation and the mechanisms involved in interrogating and modifying views. Some researchers have used this advantage to illuminate, for example, the influences on public attitudes to distribution of donor liver grafts (Wilmot and Ratcliffe, 2002). Focus group researchers are also well-placed to interrogate shifts in views over time – for example, Järvinen and Demant (2011), who traced the evolution of the views of young Danish people about drug use, through comparing data generated via focus groups held, respectively, when participants were 14–15 years, 15–16 years and 18–19 years of age. (This study is discussed in more detail in Chapter 4 in relation to research design.)
Involving semi-structured topic guides (see Chapter 6) and allowing for in-depth consideration of open-ended questions and stimulus materials, focus groups have the capacity to reflect issues and concerns salient to participants rather than closely following the researcher’s agenda. This means that the resulting data can yield surprises. For example, participants may take factors into account in their deliberations that researchers have not anticipated and this may highlight the relevance for the researcher of alternative explanations for perceptions or behaviour – or even of new theoretical frameworks that can usefully be brought to bear in analysis.
Focus groups are also especially well-suited to uncovering participants’ misconceptions and how these can arise. It is for this reason that focus groups have been used so frequently, to good advantage, in gauging the impact of health promotion campaigns. Keane et al. (1996) carried out research into African-American beliefs about immunization for infants, conceptualization of illness and efficacy of vaccines. Interestingly, the focus group discussions in the context of this study revealed that, as parents viewed fever as a primary indicator of illness, vaccines were seen as causing rather than preventing illness. Focus groups excel at identifying and exploring such misconceptions and their consequences for behaviour.
Another challenge frequently issued to focus group researchers is that of demonstrating that participants are telling us ‘the truth’. Again, this concern relates to the practice in questionnaire design of including questions with the specific purpose of cross-checking responses in order to highlight any inconsistencies. Working within the qualitative tradition, however, it is these very inconsistencies that afford the richest potential for understanding the process through which participants form their views and how they weigh up and even accommodate apparently contradictory positions. All researchers have to face the possibility that respondents are simply telling us what they think we want to hear and, when taking part in focus groups, they may also fear the disapproval of their peer group (Smithson, 2000). However, this is good news for the researcher with a particular interest in studying the mechanisms through which the peer group manages the articulation, development and negotiation of views; moreover, this is where focus groups come into their own.
Views expressed in focus groups may also be different from those expressed outside of the research context. However, holding focus groups with pre-existing teams, support groups, or friendship groups may facilitate more rounded or reasoned responses, since group members have both the opportunity and the knowledge required in order to challenge others’ accounts and ask them to account for their comments. As Wilson (1997) argues:
We will never know what respondents might have revealed in the ‘privacy’ of an in-depth interview but we do know what they were prepared to elaborate and defend in the company of their peers. (1997, p. 218)
Some researchers have waxed lyrical about the potential of focus groups to empower participants. Johnson (1996), for example, who published a paper on focus groups entitled ‘It’s good to talk’, considers that focus groups can stimulate significant changes and can lead participants to redefine their problems in a more politicized way. Focus groups have been a key component of the ‘sociological intervention’ approach developed and advocated by the French sociologist Alain Touraine (1981). The role for the sociologist, as envisioned by Touraine, reflects the now somewhat outmoded Marxist notion of the intelligentsia as heralding social change – even revolution – through spearheading social movements. This approach involved bringing people together in groups over a considerable period of time and relied on an ‘epistemology of reception’ that stresses the importance of feedback from participants elicited by presentation of sociological theory to the relevant audience. Some commentators, such as Munday (2006), have criticized Touraine’s approach as privileging the perspective of the sociologist over those who are participating in the research. However, the interests of researcher and ‘researched’ are not necessarily all that different. Gómez et al. (2011) point out that action research fits well with what they call a ‘dialogic turn’ in current societies. They argue: ‘Today people expect to participate in the wider society and discuss the issues in their lives, from families and intimate relationships to their children’s schools, their workplaces, or their city’ (p. 236). Focus groups, if used critically ‘can contribute to challenging the prevailing orthodoxy and thereby overcome established regimes of truth in the Foucauldian tradition’ (Stahl et al., 2011, p. 378). (For further discussion of Foucauldian-influenced research, see the section on philosophical and methodological traditions in Chapter 3.)
The view that focus groups engender inherently more equal relationships between researchers and researched has also led some commentators to claim that they are a feminist method. A thoughtful discussion by Wilkinson (1999), however, concludes that although focus groups are suited to addressing feminist research topics, their use does not necessarily constitute ‘feminist research’. Moreover, as Brooks (2014) points out, there may well not be such a thing as a definitive feminist methodology and researchers should be mindful that all women – even those in similar situations – do not necessarily share the same experiences of oppression, discrimination and powerlessness. As Bloor et al. (2001, p. 15) conclude: focus groups are ‘not the authentic voice of the people’ and whether or not focus groups actually ‘empower’ anyone depends on what happens after the group discussion.
We have seen, then, that both proponents and detractors of focus groups as a method, can be prone to exaggeration. Some criticisms of focus groups and their capacity for generating data and affording insights can be traced back to a lingering attachment to quantitative research assumptions, which are inappropriate when evaluating the potential of qualitative methods. Even where focus groups are used appropriately, a lack of appreciation of their full capacity can lead to them being employed in an overly casual fashion, to carry out brainstorming exercises, for example, which, although potentially illuminating, are the very least of what focus group research can achieve. Lack of preparation, piloting and refinement of topic guides have the same consequences as lack of attention to developing instruments in the quantitative tradition – suboptimal research. (This is discussed in Chapter 6 in relation to planning focus groups.)
Focus groups are an inherently flexible method and offer a wide range of options, provided that the researcher gives careful consideration to what is the most appropriate approach for the project in hand. Notwithstanding their impressive pedigree, focus groups are not always the most appropriate method. Not only does inappropriate use of focus groups result in poorly designed research; as Krueger (1993) pointed out, overzealous and inappropriate use threatens to discredit the method itself.
Making a sharp distinction between applied and more theoretical usages, however, can also be unhelpful, as it is argued that there are also many illuminating similarities and that researchers operating at each end of this continuum have much to learn from each other – particularly with regard to designing studies and capitalizing on the rich data generated.
We saw in Chapter 1 that some of the problems experienced – particularly by novice focus group researchers – stem from inappropriate expectations, arising from a lack of awareness of the particular properties of qualitative, as opposed to quantitative research. Once focus groups are placed within the context of qualitative research – and when they are viewed as a way of addressing a rather different set of questions – many of the problems and frustrations encountered by focus group researchers and the perceived weaknesses of the method can actually be shown to be advantages. The following chapter aims to provide the reader with an enhanced appreciation of focus groups as an essentially qualitative method.