Читать книгу Infants and Children in Context - Tara L. Kuther - Страница 123

Methods of Behavioral Genetics

Оглавление

Behavioral geneticists seek to estimate the heritability of specific traits and behaviors. Heritability refers to the extent to which variation among people on a given characteristic is due to genetic differences. The remaining variation not due to genetic differences is instead a result of the environment and experiences. Heritability research therefore examines the contributions of the genotype but also provides information on the role of experience in determining phenotypes (Plomin et al., 2016). Behavioral geneticists assess the hereditary contributions to behavior by conducting selective breeding and family studies.

Selective breeding studies entail deliberately modifying the genetic makeup of animals to examine the influence of heredity on attributes and behavior. For example, mice can be bred to be very physically active or sedentary by mating highly active mice only with other highly active mice and, similarly, by breeding mice with very low levels of activity with each other. Over subsequent generations, mice bred for high levels of activity become many times more active than those bred for low levels of activity (Knopik et al., 2017). Selective breeding in rats, mice, and other animals such as chickens has revealed genetic contributions to many traits and characteristics, such as aggressiveness, emotionality, sex drive, and even maze learning (Plomin et al., 2016).

For many reasons, especially ethical reasons, people cannot be selectively bred. However, we can observe people who naturally vary in shared genes and environment. Behavioral geneticists conduct family studies to compare people who live together and share varying degrees of relatedness. Two kinds of family studies are common: twin studies and adoption studies (Koenen, Amstadter, & Nugent, 2012). Twin studies compare identical and fraternal twins to estimate how much of a trait or behavior is attributable to genes. Recall that identical (monozygotic) twins share 100% of their genes because they originated from the same zygote. Like all nontwin siblings, fraternal (dizygotic) twins share 50% of their genes, as they resulted from two different fertilized ova and from two genetically different zygotes. If genes affect a given attribute, identical twins should be more similar than fraternal twins because identical twins share 100% of their genes, whereas fraternal twins share about half.

Adoption studies, on the other hand, compare the degree of similarity between adopted children and their biological parents whose genes they share (50%) and their adoptive parents with whom they share an environment but not genes. If the adopted children share similarities with their biological parents, even though they were not raised by them, it suggests that the similarities are genetic. The similarities are influenced by the environment if the children are more similar to their adoptive parents. Observations of adoptive siblings also shed light on the extent to which attributes and behaviors are influenced by the environment. For example, the degree to which two genetically unrelated adopted children reared together are similar speaks to the role of environment. Comparisons of identical twins reared in the same home with those reared in different environments can also illustrate environmental contributions to phenotypes. If identical twins reared together are more similar than those reared apart, an environmental influence can be inferred.

Infants and Children in Context

Подняться наверх