Читать книгу Business Experiments with R - B. D. McCullough - Страница 27

1.3 Case: Salk Polio Vaccine Trials

Оглавление

By the 1950s, polio had killed hundreds of thousands worldwide and infected tens of thousands per year in the United States. Many victims who did not die were condemned to spend the rest of their lives in “iron lungs” since they were unable to breathe on their own. To say nothing of the misery wrought by the disease, the effect on the community was devastating: parents kept their children indoors all summer, playgrounds were vacant, and one sick student in a class was reason for many healthy students to stay home from school. Jonas Salk developed a vaccine in 1952, and in 1954 two separate field trials were conducted, involving nearly 2 million children and 300 000 volunteers in the United States, Canada, and Finland. These constituted the largest clinical trials in history.

The first trial monitored nearly a million first, second, and third graders. The second graders were given permission slips for their parents to sign, and those children who got parental consent were vaccinated by injection. These children were compared with the non‐vaccinated first and third graders. The observant reader will have noted already that the sample is not random, and generalizing the results to the population would be problematic. Summaries are presented in Table 1.3. It appears that the vaccine worked; children who got vaccinated had a lower infection rate than children who did not get vaccinated.

Table 1.3 Results of first Salk vaccine field trial.

Group Size Rate per 100 000
Grade 2 vaccinated 225 000 25
Grades 1 and 3 725 000 54
Grade 2 no consent 125 000 44

Notice, however, that the number of second graders who did not receive permission markedly was nearly half the number who did get permission. If families who gave consent were similar to families who did not give consent, then we can assume that the difference in polio rates between the grade 2 vaccinated students and the grades 1 and 3 unvaccinated students is a good estimate of the overall effect of the vaccine at reducing polio. But are the families similar? Further analysis of the trial data showed that, on average, families who gave consent had higher incomes than families who did not give consent. What else might we deduce from this income difference, with respect to educational differences? Can we infer something about how often the children visited the family doctor and other such variables that are correlated with income and might affect the children's susceptibility to polio? How might this affect the results of the trial?

A more subtle problem arises because everybody involved in the experiment, including the physicians who monitored the children for polio, knew who had been vaccinated and who had not. Specifically, the experiment was not double‐blinded. In a blinded experiment (or single‐blind experiment), only the experimenter knows whether the subject gets the treatment or the placebo; the subject does not know. In a double‐blind experiment, neither the experimenter nor the subject knows whether the treatment or the placebo is administered. The reason that lack of blinding is a problem in this situation is that polio, when it is mild and not severe, can seem like a flu or other diseases. Physicians (who generally believe in the power of vaccines), when diagnosing a child with flu‐like symptoms, may be subconsciously biased to diagnose polio more often in the unvaccinated children than in the vaccinated children.

A formal review of this trial published in the leading statistical journal characterized the design as “stupid and futile” and the results of the trial as “worthless” (Brownlee, 1955).

To remedy these deficiencies in the first trial, a second trial focused only on children whose parents gave consent for vaccination. These children were randomly assigned to treatment and control groups, both of whom received an injection. The former received the vaccine, and the latter received a placebo saline injection. The observant reader will have noted already that there is no longer a sample selection problem. The results are presented in Table 1.4.

Table 1.4 Results of second Salk vaccine field trial.

Group Size Rate per 100 000
Treatment 200 000 28
Control 200 000 71
No consent 350 000 46

Comparing Tables 1.3 and 1.4 is instructive. Note that the incidence for vaccinated children is roughly the same, 25 vs. 28, as is the incidence for children who did not get consent, 44 vs. 46. The incidence for the “control” groups has changed markedly, and it did not go down, but it went up! Apparently the children of parents who give permission are more susceptible to polio than the children of parents who do not give permission. Thus, the first experiment was biased against the vaccine: the treatment group that got the vaccine was more susceptible to polio, while the control group that did not get the vaccine was less susceptible to polio. No one involved in the trial foresaw this possibility, though an analyst trained in the design of experiments would have been able to guard against the possibility and eliminate it, even if she couldn't foresee it! How? By randomization! We will have much more to say about randomization in subsequent chapters.

The variable “parents' propensity to give consent” is a lurking variable that is correlated with the outcome (the child getting the disease). Several mechanisms have been suggested for this correlation. For example, less educated persons are less likely to give consent, less educated persons are likely to have less income, persons with less income are less likely to live in sterile environments, and children who live in less sterile environments are more likely to have robust immune systems that offer more protection against polio.

Regardless of the true mechanism, if treatment and control had been assigned randomly, then the effects of the lurking variable would have been spread out between the two groups instead of having too many consenters in the experimental group. Thus, randomization protects against the effect of lurking variables, even when we don't know what variables we need protection against! Remember, lurking variables are not to be confused with confounding variables; again, see the “Learning More” section for details on this point.

A postscript to the experiments is in order. The success of the trials was formally announced in 1955, and widespread vaccination began. However, a problem with the injectable vaccine quickly arose. There were six different manufacturers of the vaccine, and not all of them followed Salk's instructions for preparing the vaccine. Hundreds of cases of paralysis were reported, including cases when the paralysis began in the arm that received the injection! The program was suspended in 1955. In 1960 an even better oral vaccine was introduced and put into widespread use. This is why, when examining the data, you might not see a large drop after 1955.

Business Experiments with R

Подняться наверх