Читать книгу The Handbook of Speech Perception - Группа авторов - Страница 22
Generic auditory organization and speech perception
ОглавлениеThe intelligibility of sinewave replicas of utterances, of noise‐band vocoded speech, and of speech chimeras reveals that a perceiver can find and follow a speech signal composed of dissimilar acoustic and auditory constituents, in contrast to the principles on which gestalt‐based generic functions operate. These findings show that perceptual organization of speech can occur solely by virtue of attention to the complex coordinate variation of an acoustic pattern. The use of such exotic acoustic signals for the proof creates some uncertainty that ordinary speech perception is satisfactorily characterized by tests using these acoustic oddities. An argument of Remez et al. (1994) for considering these tests to be a useful index of the perception of commonplace speech signals begins by noting that phonetic perception of sinewave replicas of utterances depends on a simple instruction to listen to the tones as speech. Because the disposition to hear sinewave words and sentences appears readily, without arduous or lengthy training, this prompt adaptation to phonetic organization and analysis suggests that the ordinary cognitive resources of speech perception are operating for sinewave speech. Although some form of short‐term perceptual learning might be involved, the swiftness of the appearance of adequate perceptual function is evidence that any special induction to accommodate sinewave signals is a marginal component of perception.
Despite all, natural speech consists of large stretches of glottal pulsing, which creates amplitude comodulation over time and harmonic relations between concurrent portions of the spectrum. This has led to a reasonable proposal (Barker & Cooke, 1999; Darwin, 2008) that generic auditory grouping functions, although not necessary for the perceptual organization of speech, contribute to perceptual organization when speech spectra satisfy the gestalt criteria. The consistent finding that speech spectra organize quickly – on the order of milliseconds – and generic auditory grouping takes time to build – on the order of seconds – may justify doubt in the asserted privilege of gestalt‐based grouping by similarity. A critical empirical test was provided by Carrell and Opie (1992), which offers an index of the plausibility of the claim. In the test, the intelligibility of sinewave sentences was compared in two acoustic conditions: (1) three‐tone time‐varying sinusoids; and (2) three‐tone time‐varying sinusoids on which a regular amplitude pulse was imposed. Although the tone patterns in the first condition were not susceptible to gestalt‐based grouping, because they failed to exhibit similarity in each of the relevant dimensions that we have discussed, the pulsed tone patterns in the second condition exhibited amplitude comodulation and harmonicity in its complex spectra (Bregman, Levitan, & Liao, 1990). All other things being equal, the perceptual organization attributable to complex coordinate variation should have been reinforced by perceptual organization attributable to similarity that triggers generic auditory grouping. Indeed, Carrell and Opie found that pulsed sentences were more intelligible than smoothly varying sinusoids, as if the spectral components once bound more securely were more successfully analyzed.
The assertion offered by Barker and Cooke (1999) about this phenomenon is that generic auditory functions can reinforce the grouping of speech signals, although on close examination the evidence does not yet warrant an endorsement of a hybrid model of perceptual organization. Carrell and Opie had used a range of pulse rates and conditions in their study, and reported that the intelligibility gain attributable to pulsing a sinewave sentence was restricted to a pulse rate in the range of 50–100 Hz. No benefit of pulsing was observed for a pulse rate of 200 Hz. While this topic merits additional examination, the available evidence encourages a doubtful conclusion about this hypothetical hybrid character of perceptual organization, which would necessarily be limited in applicability to speech signals produced by low bass voices; its benefit would not extend to tenors, to say nothing of altos and sopranos. Most generously, we might conclude that the relation of primitive gestalt‐based generic auditory grouping and the more abstract organization by sensitivity to coordinate variation cannot be defined without stronger evidence, and that it is premature to conclude that the gestalt set plays a prominent or even a secondary role in the perceptual organization of speech.