Читать книгу Security Engineering - Ross Anderson - Страница 71

3.2.2 Gender, diversity and interpersonal variation

Оглавление

Many women die because medical tests and technology assume that patients are men, or because engineers use male crash-test dummies when designing cars; protective equipment, from sportswear through stab-vests to spacesuits, gets tailored for men by default [498]. So do we have problems with information systems too? They are designed by men, and young geeky men at that, yet over half their users may be women. This realisation has led to research on gender HCI – on how software should be designed so that women can also use it effectively. Early experiments started from the study of behaviour: experiments showed that women use peripheral vision more, and it duly turned out that larger displays reduce gender bias. Work on American female programmers suggested that they tinker less than males, but more effectively [203]. But how much is nature, and how much nurture? Societal factors matter, and US women who program appear to be more thoughtful, but lower self-esteem and higher risk-aversion leads them to use fewer features.

Gender has become a controversial topic in psychology research. In the early 2000s, discussion of male aptitude for computer science was sometimes in terms of an analysis by Simon Baron-Cohen which gives people separate scores as systemisers (good at geometry and some kinds of symbolic reasoning) and as empathisers (good at intuiting the emotions of others and social intelligence generally) [177]. Most men score higher at systematising, while most women do better at empathising. The correspondence isn't exact; a minority of men are better at empathising while a minority of women are better at systematising. Baron-Cohen's research is in Asperger's and autism spectrum disorder, which he sees as an extreme form of male brain. This theory gained some traction among geeks who saw an explanation of why we're often introverted with more aptitude for understanding things than for understanding people. If we're born that way, it's not out fault. It also suggests an explanation for why geek couples often have kids on the spectrum.

Might this explain why men are more interested in computer science than women, with women consistently taking about a sixth of CS places in the USA and the UK? But here, we run into trouble. Women make up a third of CS students in the former communist countries of Poland, Romania and the Baltic states, while numbers in India are close to equal. Male dominance of software is also a fairly recent phenomenon. When I started out in the 1970s, there were almost as many women programmers as men, and many of the pioneers were women, whether in industry, academia or government. This suggests that the relevant differences are more cultural than genetic or developmental. The argument for a ‘male brain / female brain’ explanation has been progressively undermined by work such as that of Daphna Joel and colleagues who've shown by extensive neuroimaging studies that while there are recognisable male and female features in brains, the brains of individuals are a mosaic of both [987]. And although these features are visible in imaging, that does not mean they're all laid down at birth: our brains have a lot of plasticity. As with our muscles the tissues we exercise grow bigger. Perhaps nothing else might have been expected given the variance in gender identity, sexual preference, aggression, empathy and so on that we see all around us.

Other work has shown that gender performance differences are absent in newborns, and appear round about age 6–7, by which time children have long learned to distinguish gender and adapt to the social cues all around them, which are reinforced in developed countries by a tsunami of blue/pink gendered toys and marketing. (Some believe that women are happier to work in computing in India because India escaped the home computer boom in the 1980s and its evolution into gaming.) This is reinforced in later childhood and adolescence by gender stereotypes that they internalise as part of their identity; in cultures where girls aren't supposed to be good at maths or interested in computers, praise for being ‘good at maths’ can evoke a stereotype threat (the fear of confirming a negative stereotype about a group to which one belongs). Perhaps as a result, men react better to personal praise (‘That was really clever of you!’) while women are motivated better by performance praise (‘You must have put in a hell of a lot of effort’). So it may not be surprising that we see a deficit of women in disciplines that praise genius, such as mathematics. What's more, similar mechanisms appear to underlie the poorer academic performance of ethnic groups who have been stigmatised as non-academic. In short, people are not just born different; we learn to be different, shaped by power, by cultural attitudes, by expectations and by opportunities. There are several layers between gene and culture with emergent behaviour, including the cell and the circuit. So if we want more effective interventions in the pipeline from school through university to professional development, we need a better understanding of the underlying neurological and cultural mechanisms. For a survey of this, see Gina Rippon [1608].

Gender matters at many levels of the stack, from what a product should do through how it does it. For example, should a car be faster or safer? This is entangled with social values. Are men better drivers because they win car races, or are women better drivers because they have fewer insurance claims? Digging down, we find gendered and cultural attitudes to risk. In US surveys, risks are judged lower by white people and by men, and on closer study this is because about 30% of white males judge risks to be extremely low. This bias is consistent across a wide range of hazards but is particularly strong for handguns, second-hand cigarette smoke, multiple sexual partners and street drugs. Asian males show similarly low sensitivity to some hazards, such as motor vehicles. White males are more trusting of technology, and less of government [693].

We engineers must of course work with the world as it is, not as it might be if our education system and indeed our culture had less bias; but we must be alert to the possibility that computer systems discriminate because they are built by men for men, just like cars and spacesuits. For example, Tyler Moore and I did an experiment to see whether anti-phishing advice given by banks to their customers was easier for men to follow than women, and we found that indeed it was [1339]. No-one seems to have done much work on gender and security usability, so there's an opportunity.

But the problem is much wider. Many systems will continue to be designed by young fit straight clever men who are white or Asian and may not think hard or at all about the various forms of prejudice and disability that they do not encounter directly. You need to think hard about how you mitigate the effects. It's not enough to just have your new product tested by a token geek girl on your development team; you have to think also of the less educated and the vulnerable – including older people, children and women fleeing abusive relationships (about which I'll have more to say later). You really have to think of the whole stack. Diversity matters in corporate governance, market research, product design, software development and testing. If you can't fix the imbalance in dev, you'd better make it up elsewhere. You need to understand your users; it's also good to understand how power and culture feed the imbalance.

As many of the factors relevant to group behaviour are of social origin, we next turn to social psychology.

Security Engineering

Подняться наверх